Смекни!
smekni.com

Теоретическая грамматика английского языка 2 (стр. 40 из 54)

For instance, a question can be described as transformationally produced from a statement; a negation, likewise, can be presented as transformationally produced front an affirmation. E.g.:

You are fond of the kid.Are you fond of the kid?

You are fond of the kid. You are not fond of the kid.

Why are the directions of transitions given in this way and not vice versa? - Simply because the ordinary affirmative statement pre­sents a positive expression of a fact in its purest form, maximally free of the speaker's connotative appraisals.

Similarly, a composite sentence, for still more evident reasons, is to be presented as derived from two or more simple sentences. E.g.:

He turned to the waiter. + The waiter stood in the door. He turned to the waiter who stood in the door.

These transitional relations are implicitly inherent in the syntag­matic classificational study of sentences. But modern theory, exposing them explicitly, has made a cardinal step forward in so far as it has interpreted them as regular derivation stages comparable to categorial form-making processes in morphology and word-building.

And it is on these lines that the initial, basic clement of syntac­tic derivation has been found, i.e. a syntactic unit serving as a "sentence-root" and providing an objective ground for identifying syntactic categorial oppositions. This element is known by different names, such as the "basic syntactic pattern", the "structural sentence scheme", the "elementary sentence model", the "base sentence", though as the handiest in linguistic use should be considered the "kernel sentence" due to its terminological flexibility combined with a natural individualizing force.

Structurally the kernel sentence coincides with the elementary sentence described in the previous chapter. The difference is, that the pattern of the kernel sentence is interpreted as forming the base of a paradigmatic derivation in the corresponding sentence pattern se­ries.

Thus, syntactic derivation should not be understood as an imme­diate change of one sentence into another one; a pronounced or written sentence is a finished utterance that thereby cannot undergo any changes. Syntactic derivation is to be understood as paradigmatic production of more complex pattern constructions out of kernel pat­tern constructions as their structural bases. The description of this production ("generation") may be more detailed and less detailed, i.e. it can be effected in more generalized and less generalized terms, depending on the aim of the scholar. The most concrete presentation concerns a given speech utterance analysed into its derivation history on the level of the word-forms.

By way of example let us take the following English sentence: I saw him come.

This sentence is described in scholar grammar as a sentence with a complex object, which is syntagmatically adequate, though incom­plete from the systemic point of view. The syntagmatic description is supplemented and re-interpreted within the framework of the paradigmatic description presenting the sentence in question as pro­duced from the two kernel sentences: Isaw him. + He came. I saw him come.

In a more generalized, categorial-oriented paradigmatic presenta­tion the sentence will be shown as a transformational combination of the two kernel pattern-formulas:

The same may be given in terms of the IC-derivation tree dia­grams (see Fig. 8). The indices specifying the basic symbols can vary in accord with the concrete needs of analysis and demonstration.

§ 3. The derivation of genuine sentences lying on the "surface" of speech out of kernel sentences lying in the "deep base" of speech can be analysed as a process falling into sets of elementary trans­formational steps or procedures. These procedures make up six ma­jor classes.

The first class includes steps of "morphological arrangement" ofthe sentence, i.e. morphological changes expressing syntactically rele­vant categories, above all, the predicative categories of the finite verb: tense, aspect, voice, mood. The syntactic role of these forms of morphological change (systematized into morphological paradigms) consists in the fact that they make up parts of the more general syntactico-paradigmatic series. E.g.:

John + start (the kernel base string) John starts. John will be starting. John would be starting. John has started. Etc.

The second class of the described procedures includes various uses of functional words (functional expansion). From the syntactic point of view these words are transformers of syntactic constructions in the same sense as the categorial morphemes (e.g. inflexions) are transformers of lexemes, i.e. morphological constructions. E.g.:

He understood my request. He seemed to understand my re­quest. Now they consider the suggestion.Now they do consider the suggestion.

The third class of syntactic derivational procedures includes the processes of substitution. Among the substitutes we find personal pronouns, demonstrative-substitute pronouns, indefinite-substitute pronouns, as well as substitutive combinations of half-notional words. Cf.:

The pupils ran out ofthe classroom. They ran out of the classroom. 1 want another pen, please. I want another one, please.

The fourth class of the procedures in question is formed by pro­cesses of deletion, i.e. elimination of some elements of the sentence in various contextual conditions. As a result of deletion the corre­sponding reduced constructions are produced. E.g:.

Would you like a cup of tea? A cup of tea? It's a pleasure! Pleasure!

The liith class of syntactic derivational procedures includes pro­cesses of positional arrangement, in particular, permutations (changes of the word-order into the reverse patterns). E.g.:

The man is here. Is the man here? Jim ran in with an ex­cited cry. In ran Jim with an excited cry.

The sixth class of syntactic derivational procedures is formed by processes of intonattonal arrangement, i.e. application of various functional tones and accents. This arrangement is represented in written and typed speech by punctuation marks, the use of different varieties of print, the use of various modes of underlining and other graphical means. E.g.:

We must go. We must go? We? Must go?? You care noth­ing about what I feel. You care nothing about what / feel!

The described procedures are all functionally relevant, i.e. they serve as syntactically meaningful dynamic features of the sentence. For various expressive purposes they may be applied either singly or, more often than not, in combination with one another. E.g.:

We finish the workWe are not going to finish it.

For the production of the cited sentence-transform the following procedures are used: morphological change, introduction of functional words, substitution, intonational arrangement. The functional (meaningful) outcome of the whole process is the expression of the modal future combined with a negation in a dialogue response. Cf.:

Are we ever going to finish the work? Anyway, we are not going to finish it today!

§ 4. The derivational procedures applied to the kernel sentence introduce it into two types of derivational relations in the sentential paradigmatic system: first, the "constructional" relations; second, the "predicative" relations. The constructional derivation effects the for­mation of more complex clausal structures out of simpler ones; in other words, it provides for the expression of the nominative-notional syntactic semantics of the sentence. The predicative derivation realizes the formation of predicatively different units not affecting the con­structional volume of the base; in other words, it is responsible for the expression of the predicative syntactic semantics of the sentence. Both types of derivational procedures form the two subsystems within the general system of syntactic paradigmatics.

§ 5. As part of the constructional system of syntactic paradig­matics, kernel sentences, as well as other, expanded base-sentences undergo derivational changes into clauses and phrases.

The transformation of a base sentence into a clause can be called "clausalization". By way of clausalization a sentence is changed into a subordinate or coordinate clause in the process of subordinative or coordinative combination of sentences. The main clausalizing procedures involve the use of conjunctive words - subordinators and coordinators. Since a composite sentence is produced from minimum two base sentences, the derivational processes of composite sentence production are sometimes called "two-base transformations".

For example, two kernel sentences They arrived and They re­lieved me of my fears (Iwas relieved of my fears), combined by subordinative and coordinative clausalizing, produce the following constructions:

When they arrived I was relieved of my fears. If they ar­rive, I shall be relieved of my fears. Even though they arrive, I shan't be relieved of my fears. Etc. They arrived, and I was relieved of my fears. They arrived, but I was not relieved of my fears. Etc.

The transformation of a base sentence into a phrase can be called "phrasalization". By phrasalization a sentence is transformed either into a semi-predicative construction (a semi-clause), or into a nominal phrase.

Nominal phrases are produced by the process of nominalization, i.e. nominalizing phrasalization which we have analyzed before (see Ch. XX). Nominalization may be complete, consisting in completely depriving the sentence, of its predicative aspect, or partial, consisting in partially depriving the sentence of its predicative aspect. Partial nominalization in English produces infinitive and gerundial phrases. By other types of phrasalization such semi-clauses are derived as complex objects of infinitive and participial types, various participial constructions of adverbial status and some other, minor complexes. The resulting constructions produced by the application of the cited phrasalizing procedures in the process of derivational combination of base sentences will be both simple expanded sentences (in case of complete nominalization) and semi-composite sentences (in case of various partial nominalizations and other phrasalizations). Cf:.

On their arrival I was relieved of my fears. They arrived to relieve me of my fears.They arrived relieving me of my fears.Having arrived, they did relieve me of my fears. Etc.

As is seen from the examples, each variety of derivational com­bination of concrete sentences has its own semantic purpose ex­pressed by the procedures employed.

§ 6. As part of the predicative system of syntactic paradigmatics, kernel sentences, as well as expanded base-sentences, undergo such structural modifications as immediately express the predicative func­tions of the sentence, i.e. the functions relating the nominative meanings of the sentence to reality. Of especial importance in this respect is the expression of predicative functions by sentences which are elementary as regards the set of their notional constituents: being elementary from the point of view of nominative semantics, these sentences can be used as genuine, ordinary utterances of speech. Bearing in mind the elementary nominative nature of its construc­tional units, we call the system of sentences so identified the "Primary Syntactic System" (Lat. "Prima Systema Syntactica").

To recognize a primary sentence in the text, one must use the criteria of elementary sentence structure identification applied to the notional constituents of the sentence, irrespective of the functional meanings rendered by it. For instance, the notionally minimal nega­tive sentence should be classed as primary, though not quite ele­mentary (kernel) in the paradigmatic sense, negation being not a no­tional, but a functional sentence factor. Cf.:

I have met the man. I have not met the man. I have never met the man.

Any composite (or semi-composite) sentence is analysable into two or more primary sentences (i.e. sentences elementary in the no­tional sense). E.g.:

Is it a matter of no consequence that I should find you with a young man wearing my pyjamas? Is it a matter of no conse­quence? + I should find you with a (young) man. + The (young) man is wearing my pyjamas.

The kernel sentence can also have its representation in speech, being embodied by the simplest sentential construction not only in the notional, but also in the functional sense. In other words, it is an elementary sentence which is non-interrogative, non-imperative, non-negative, non-modal, etc. In short, in terms of syntactic opposi­tions, this is the "weakest" construction in the predicative oppositional space of the primary syntactic system.

§ 7. The predicative functions expressed by primary sentence patterns should be divided into the two types: first, lower functions; second, higher functions. The lower functions include the expression of such morphological categories as tenses and aspects; these are of "factual", "truth-stating" semantic character. The higher functions are "evaluative" in the broad sense of the word; they immediately ex­press the functional semantics of relating the nominative content of the sentence to reality.

The principal predicative functions expressed by syntactic categorial oppositions are the following.

First, question as opposed to statement. Second, inducement as opposed to statement. Third, negation as opposed to affirmation. Fourth, unreality as opposed to reality. Fifth, probability as opposed to fact. Sixth, modal identity (seem to do, happen to do, prove to do, etc.) as opposed to fact. Seventh, modal subject-action relation as opposed to fact (cando, may do, etc.). Eighth, specified actual subject-action relation as opposed to fact. Ninth, phase of action as opposed to fact. Tenth, passive action as opposed to active action. Eleventh, specialized actual division (specialized perspective) as op­posed to non-specialized actual division (non-specialized perspective). Twelfth, emphasis (emotiveness) as opposed to emotional neutrality (unemotiveness).

Each opposition of the cited list forms a categorial set which is rather complex. For instance, within the framework of the ques­tion - statement opposition, pronominal and alternative questions are identified with their manifold varieties; within the system of phase of action, specialized subsets are identified rendering the phase of be­ginning, the phase of duration, the phase of end, etc. The total supersystem of all the pattern-forms of a given sentence base consti­tutes its general syntactic paradigm of predicative functions. This paradigm is, naturally, extremely complicated so that it is hardly ob­servable if presented on a diagram. This fact shows that the volume of functional meanings rendered by a sentence even at a very high level of syntactic generalization is tremendous. At the same time the derivation of each functional sentence form in its paradigmatically determined position in the system is simple enough in the sense that it is quite explicit. This shows the dynamic essence of the paradigm in question; the paradigm exactly answers the needs of expression at every given juncture of actual communication,

§ 8. All the cited oppositions-categories may or may not be rep­resented in a given utterance by their strong function members. In accord with this oppositional regularity, we advance the notion of the "predicative load" of the sentence. The predicative load is determined by the total volume of the strong members of predicative oppositions (i.e. by the sum of positive values of the corresponding differential features) actually represented in the sentence.

The sentence, by definition, always expresses predication, being a predicative unit of language. But, from the point of view of the comparative volume of the predicative meanings actually expressed, the sentence may be predicatively "loaded" or "non-loaded". If the sentence is predicatively "non-loaded", it means that its construction is kernel elementary at the accepted level of categorial generalization. Consequently, such a sentence will be characterized in oppositional terms as non-interrogative, non-inducive, non-negative, non-real, non probable, non-modal-identifying, etc., down to the last of the recog­nized predicative oppositions. If, on the other hand, the sentence is predicatively "loaded", it means that it renders at least one of the strong oppositional meanings inherent in the described categorial system. Textual observations show that predicative loads amounting to one or two positive feature values (strong oppositional members) may be characterized as more or less common; hence, we consider such a load as "light" and, correspondingly, say that the sentence in this case is predicatively "lightly" loaded. As for sentences whose predicative load exceeds two positive feature values, they stand out of the common, their functional semantics showing clear signs of in­tricacy. Accordingly, we consider such loads as "heavy", and of sen­tences characterized by these loads we say that they are "heavily" loaded. Predicative loads amounting to four feature values occur but occasionally, they are too complicated to be naturally grasped by the mind.