Смекни!
smekni.com

Учебно-методическое пособие к курсу «лингвострановедение и страноведение» для студентов старших курсов (стр. 5 из 19)

Independence or identity

But how close is this relationship between language and thought. It is usual to see this question in terms of two extremes. First, there is the hypothesis that language and thought are totally separate entities, with one being dependent on the other. At the opposite extreme, there is the hypothesis that language and thought are identical-that it is not possible to engage in any rational thinking without using language. The truth seems to lie somewhere between these two positions.

Within the first position, there are plainly two possibilities: language might be dependent upon thought, or thought might be dependent upon language. The traditional view, which is widely held at a popular level, adopts the first of these: people have thoughts, and then they put these thoughts into words. It is summarized in such metaphorical views of language as the "dress" or "tool" of thought. The view is well represented in the field of child language acquisition, where children are seen to develop a range of cognitive abilities which precede the learning of language.

The second possibility has also been widely held: the way people use language dictates the lines along which they can think. An expressive summary of this is Shelley's "He gave men speech, and speech created thought," which is the measure of the universe (Prometheus Unbound). This view is also represented in the language acquisition field, in the argument that the child's earliest encounters with language are the main influence on the way concepts are learned. The most influential expression' of this position, however, is found in the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis.

A third possibility, which is also, widely held these days, is that language and thought are independent-but this is not to say that they are identical. The identity view (for example, that thought is no more than an internalized vocalization) is no longer common. There are too many exceptions for such a strong position to maintain: we need think only of the various kinds of mental operations which we can perform without language, such as recalling a sequence of movements in a game or sport, or visualizing the route from home to work. It is also widely recognized that pictorial images and physical models are helpful in problem-solving, and may at times be more efficient than purely verbal representations of a problem.

On the other hand, these cases are far outnumbered by those where language does seem to be the main means whereby successful thinking can proceed. To see language and thought as independent, then, is so recognize that language is a regular part of the process of thinking, at the same time recognizing that we have to think in order to understand language. It is not a question of one notion taking precedence over the other, but of both notions being essential, if we are to explain behavior. Once again, people have searched for metaphors to express their views. Language has been likened to the arch of a tunnel: thought, to the tunnel itself. But the complex structure and function of language defies simple analogies.

3. The Sapir-Whorf hypothesis

The romantic idealism of the late 18th century as encountered in the views of Johann Herder (1774-1803) and Wilhelm von Humboldt (1762-1835), placed great value on the diversity of the world's languages and cultures. The tradition was taken up by the American linguist and anthropologist Edward Sapir (1884-1939) and his pupil Benjamin Lee Whorf (1897-1941) and resulted in a view about the relation between languages and thought which was widely influential in the middle decades of this century. The "Sapir-Whorf hypothesis", as it came to be called, combines two principles. The first is known as linguistic determinism: it states that language determines the way we think. The second follows from this, and known as linguistic relativity: it states that the distinctions encoded in one language are not found in any other language. In a much-quoted paragraph, Whorf propounds the view as follow: "We dissect nature along lines laid down by our native languages. The categories and types that we isolate from the world of phenomena we do not find there because they stare every observer in the face; on the contrary, the world is presented in a kaleidoscopic flux of impressions which has to be organized by our minds-and this means largely by the linguistic systems in our minds. We cut nature up, organize it into concepts, and ascribe significances as we do, largely because we are parties to an agreement to organize it in this way-an agreement that holds throughout our speech community and is codified in the patterns of our languages. The agreement is, of course, an implicit and unstated one, but its terms are absolutely obligatory; we cannot talk at all except by subscribing to the organization and classification of data which the agreement decrees". However, a weaker version of the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis is generally accepted. Language may not determine the way we think, but it does influence the way we perceive and remember, and it affects the ease with which we perform mental tasks. Several experiments have shown that people recall things more easily if the things correspond to readily available words or phrases. And people certainly find it easier to make a conceptual distinction if it nearly corresponds to words available in their language.

Aids to study the text:

1. What is language? Give several definitions to illustrate its essence.

2. Describe the interrelation between language and thinking.

3. Why is language considered the most important means of human communication? Give your arguments.

4. Comment on the various approaches to the defining of the functions of language.

5. Expand on the functions of language as pointed out by the English linguist D.Chrystal.

6. Dwell on the Sapir – Worf hypothesis and illustrate its essence.

Chapter 2. The system of Indo-European languages

1. General characteristics of Indo-European languages

It has been estimated that there are more than 2,700 distinct languages to be found in the world today, and all these fall into linguistic groups, which are part of linguistic families which may have appeared in different parts of the globe simultaneously.

It should be borne in mind that when people speak of linguistic families they do not use the term "family" in its genetic sense. The fact that people speak the same, or related, languages does not mean that there is a link of race or blood between them. It is therefore completely unscientific to establish any connection between racial origin and language.

It is often possible to show that languages are historically or genetically related, i.e. they descend from a common source, but when it comes to races we have no such an evidence. We cannot say, for instance, that the Mongolian race means the same as the Mongolian languages. Furthermore, it is quite probable that the Indo-European race never existed. In the course of the migrations of ancient peoples, numerous linguistic and racial mixtures took place. The linguistic map of the world shows that many non-Indo-European peoples of Europe and Asia ceased using their own languages and adopted the Indo-European languages. The Basque language, which is spoken in the north of Spain and the south of France, resisted the assimilation of Indo-European in the past and, thus, it is not genetically related to the Indo-European languages. On the other hand, there is no racial difference between the Estonians, for instance, who speak a Finno-Ugric language, and the Letts, who speak a language of Indo-European origin.

So, all the attempts to draw a parallel between race and language which were put forward at the end of the 19th century by chauvinistically-minded linguists were sharply criticized by progressive thinkers. In trying to reconstruct the original state of any linguistic family, linguists face many difficulties. The vital problem was the absence of any recorded history of languages entering the family on the one hand, and the vast language migrations on the other. Tribal migrations, which took place in the distant past, completely obscured the linguistic state of antiquity and resulted in disappearance of whole peoples and emergence of new tribes with their own languages.

But in considering the great migrations and the prehistory of language, we shall take as an example the Indo-European family, because a lot of information has been obtained about this linguistic group through the thorough work of investigators in many countries over a long period of time.

The name given to this family of languages, Indo-European, is based on the fact that it covered most of Europe and extended eastward as far as northern India. The people speaking this original language lived about 2,500 to 2,000 B. C.

In the 19th century, the original home of the Indo-European people was supposed to be situated in Central Asia, and that successive waves of emigration from there carried the various members of the family to Europe. This can be explained by the confusion of the primitive Aryans with the much earlier Indo-Europeans, and by the importance attached to the oldest Indo-European language, Sanskrit.

Recent researches show that it is possible to narrow down the territorial limits in Europe, within which the cradle of the Indo-European languages is to be found. It is known with reasonable certainty that the Italian and Greek peninsulas were colonized from the North. The occupation of France and the British Isles by the Celts from Central Europe took place comparatively late (500 B. C). The Iberian Peninsula remained predominantly non-Indo-European until Roman times, and in modern Basque a trace of pre-Indo-European speech still survives. The Eastern limit is indicated by the fact that before the two Asiatic migrations (Tocharian and Indo-Iranian), Indo-European must have been bordered to the east by an early form of Finno-Ugric, and there is some evidence of contact between these two families in the primitive period. There is reason to believe that the original centre of Finno-Ugrian expansion lay between the Volga and the Urals, and this gives us the furthest boundary, beyond which Indo-European was not to be found in its early stages. This leaves the central part of Europe, extending from the Rhine to Central and Southern Russia, and the greater part of this area had long been occupied by various Indo-European dialects. Some linguists consider that it is impossible to define the original Indo-European homeland to limits any narrower than these.

The information we have obtained about the Indo-European language-family is based mainly on linguistic evidence. The Indo-European vocabulary reveals a great deal in this respect, which is not surprising when one considers that if a single word occurs in all branches of the Indo-European family, it can be reasonably assumed that it descends from the original language. If that happens repeatedly with words of a certain type, it can be understood that whatever those words describe, they are part of the original Indo-European language. On the contrary, if certain types of words bear no likenesses in the Indo-European languages, it follows that the material circumstances which brought these words into being came relatively late. For instance, most Indo-European languages have common words for animals like bears and wolves, for plants like pine-trees, for phenomena like snow. But there are no common words for elephants, crocodiles, or palm trees.

Due to the analysis of these linguistic clues, it becomes possible to draw a vivid image of the Indo-European peoples as a race, concerning their way and conditions of life. Thus, these peoples lived in forests far from lakes, rivers or seas, because the same words naming such trees as birches, willows, and oak-trees can be found in all Indo-European languages. They had domestic animals like the horse, dog, sheep, pig, goose. At prehistoric times Indo-Europeans were apparently cattle-raising nomads and had a stone-age culture. Their instruments were mostly of stone, but they made some use of metals as well. Their religion is supposed to have been pantheistic, i.e. worshiping of Sky-father and Earth-mother. The Indo-Europeans must have been ardent and violent warriors for the considerable number successful conquests.

Thorough investigation of the customs and traditions of the oldest descendants of the Indo-European peoples enabled the scientists to arrive at fundamental conclusions concerning their social organization. Thus, the use of cattle for money was found among the early Slavonic peoples, the Irish and the early Romans.

The comparative method allows us to state that Proto-Indo-European (PIE) was a highly inflective language. Nouns and verbs were characterized by rich paradigmatic variations. Nouns had at least eight case-forms— nominative, vocative, accusative, genitive, dative, locative, ablative, and instrumental. Many suffixes were involved in the formation of the Verb paradigm. Both nouns and verbs had distinct forms for the dual number. The forms of the pronouns already showed different roots, like me, and we, us in English. There were no separate inflexions for the passive, but only for the middle voice, which expressed the idea that the speaker was especially interested in the action denoted by the verb. The word order within the Indo-European sentence was free like in Greek and Latin. Subject, verb and object could occupy the primary position; attribute usually preceded substantive, as in “good man”, for example. Counting was based on ten; nevertheless traces of the duodecimal system remained. Whereas numerals from one to four were felt to function as adjectives, those above four were taken as nouns.

Shortly after 2,000 B. C. the Indo-Europeans, supressed by other tribes, had to make great migrations, and they began to spread in different directions. Some of them moved as far as south-eastern Asia, entering the Indian Peninsula through the Khyber Pass in the 2nd millennium B. C., probably before 1,500 B. C. This group spoke a language, which became known at a later stage as Sanskrit. On their way, these Indo-Europeans split up to such an extent so that to leave several related languages scattered along their route, in Afghanistan, Baluchistan, and modern Iran.

One section seems to have gone directly westward, then down into the Balkan Peninsula, arriving at the coast of the Ionian Sea and giving us classical and then modern Greek. The Italic people were pushed from the Alps southwards. The Proto-Germans followed the Celts and left their languages all over northern Europe.

It should be borne in mind that the Indo-European speech on reaching Europe was granted no opportunity to spread across this area freely: it had to come in contact with Basque, Etruscan and other languages predominating on this territory.

It is certain that the Indo-European was not so monolithic a language as to be fully reconstructed by comparison. In 1880s, linguists revealed the existence of different dialects within the Indo-European parent language. At present we cannot do much about these differences; but it is important to recognize their existence. A period of dialect divergence preceded the final separation of the Indo-European languages from their parent stock, and these dialects had created separate languages even before the period of the great migrations.

The family of Indo-European languages as a whole is subdivided into ten major branches, in addition to which there used to have been others which died out without leaving any written records. The ten major branches and their main representatives are as follows: Indo-Iranian, which was later subdivided into:

I. Indian (its oldest form is Sanskrit). The main representatives of the modern Indian languages include Bengali, Marathi, Hindi, Gipsy and some others).

II. Iranian, which is represented by such languages as Avestan or Zend ( the old form), the so-called Pahlavi (the middle form) and Baluchi, Pushtu, Kurdish, Yagnobi, Ossetic, and some other modern languages.

III. Baltic, which is divided into Lithuanian (the language spoken by some three million people in the Lithuania), the old texts of which go back to the 16th century, and Lettish, spoken by 2 million people.

IV. The Slavonic languages, which are subdivided into three large groups:

1. Eastern Slavonic where we find three languages:

a) Russian, spoken by more than 122 million people, the basis of a common and a literary language;

b) Ukrainian, called Little Russian before the 1917 Revolution, spoken by some 40 million people;

c) Byelorussian (white Russian), spoken by 9 million people.

2. Southern Slavonic which include:

a) Bulgarian, current mostly in Bulgaria among more than seven million people;

b) Serbo-Croatian, the language of the Serbs and Croats, about 12 million people, chiefly on the territory of former Yugoslavia, whose oldest texts date from the 11th century;

c) Slovenian, spoken by 2 million people, with its oldest texts dating from the 10th century.

3. Western Slavonic, the main representatives of which are:

a) Czech, used by about 10 million people in Czechoslovakia, with texts going back to the 13th century;